The consensus on consensus messaging

A scientist would never tolerate statements about climate change that weren't based on scientific research and empirical evidence. However, the same evidentiary standards don't always seem to apply to statements about how to communicate about climate change. For example, on the topic of communicating the scientific consensus on human-caused global warming, there are lots of opinions on whether communicating the scientific consensus is effective or not. Many of these opinions are not based on the body of empirical research into consensus messaging. 

So this post is a summary of the empirical research into consensus messaging and how people think about consensus. In psychological research, public perception of scientific consensus has been found to be so important that researchers now describe this dynamic as the Gateway Belief Model. This model finds that communicating the scientific consensus on climate change increased beliefs about climate change, which subsequently increases public support for climate policy. The vast majority of research in this field either confirms the Gateway Belief Model or confirms the efficacy of consensus messaging.

The following table lists any papers that test either consensus messaging or the Gateway Belief Model. Studies are either correlational (surveys that explore whether there's an association between perceived consensus and other climate beliefs) or experimental (randomized tests that measure the impact of consensus messaging). The Country column lists which countries the participants come from. The Support column indicates whether the study supports the Gateway Belief Model or the positive effect of consensus messaging.

Table: Studies into the Gateway Belief Model or Consensus Messaging

  Author (year) Study type Country Finding Support
1 Malka et al. (2009) Correlational USA Perceived consensus mediates association of knowledge with climate concern among Democrats and Independents who trust scientists. Y
2 Ding et al. (2011) Correlational USA Low perceived consensus is associated with lower climate beliefs and lower policy support. Y
3 Lewandowsky et al. (2013) Experimental Australia Consensus messaging increases acceptance of AGW. Y
4 Rolfe-Redding et al. (2011) Correlational USA Perceived consensus predicts climate beliefs and attitudes among Republicans. Y
5 McCright et al. (2013) Correlational USA Perceived consensus affects policy support, mediated by global warming beliefs. Y
6 Aklin & Urpelainen (2014) Experimental USA Modest amounts of scientific dissent undermine public support for environmental policy. Y
7 Bolsen et al. (2014) Experimental USA Consensus messaging reduces partisan differences on behavioral intent and belief in AGW. Y
8 van der Linden et al. (2014) Experimental USA Consensus messaging (in pie-chart form) reduces partisan difference in perceived consensus. Y
9 Myers et al. (2015) Experimental USA Consensus messaging is equally effective among liberals and conservatives. Y
10 van der Linden et al. (2015) Experimental USA Increasing perceived consensus is significantly and causally associated with climate beliefs, which predicts increased policy support. Y
11 Cook & Lewandowsky (2016) Experimental Australia, USA Consensus messaging reduces partisan differences on belief in AGW for Australians. It increases partisan differences for Americans but still have an overall positive effect on belief in AGW. Y
12 Deryugina & Shurchkov (2016) Experimental USA Consensus messaging increases acceptance of climate change and human causation. Y
13 Hamilton (2016) Correlational USA Acceptance of AGW correlates with perceived consensus. Y
14 Hornsey et al. (2016) Correlational USA, UK, Australia, 30 European countries Perceived consensus is a strong predictor of belief in climate change (stronger than cultural cognition). Y
15 Schuldt & Pearson (2016) Correlational USA Perceived consensus is associated with mitigation support for both whites and non-whites. Y
16 Brewer & McKnight (2017) Experimental USA Comedy segment about consensus has strongest effect on belief in climate change among participants with low interest in the environment. Y
17 Cook et al. (2017) Experimental USA Consensus messaging neutralizes polarizing influence of misinformation. Y
18 Dixon et al. (2017) Experimental USA Consensus messaging does not produce significant effects (including no backfire effect among conservatives). Neutral
19 van der Linden et al. (2017a) Experimental USA Consensus messaging reduces partisan differences on perceived consensus. Y
20 Bolsen & Druckman (2018a) Experimental USA Consensus messaging backfires with conspiracy theorists, but consensus messaging coupled with belief validation increases acceptance of AGW among conspiracy theorists. Neutral
21 Bolsen & Druckman (2018b) Experimental USA Consensus message increases perceived consensus with indirect effect on belief in AGW and policy support. Y
22 Harris et al. (2018) Experimental UK Consensus messaging increases perceived consensus and climate beliefs. Y
23 Kerr & Wilson (2018a) Correlational New Zealand Perceived consensus does not predict later personal climate beliefs. N
24 Kerr & Wilson (2018b) Experimental New Zealand Consensus messaging increases perceived consensus with indirect effect on belief in AGW. Y
25 Kobayashi (2018b)

Correlational, Experimental

Japan Perceived consensus predicts climate beliefs. Consensus messaging increases climate beliefs through perceived consensus.   Y
26  Tom (2018) Correlational USA  Misconception about consensus is one of the most important factors in predicting scientifically deviant beliefs.  Y
27 van der Linden et al. (2018b) Correlational USA Perceived consensus did predict later personal climate beliefs.  Y
28 Zhang et al. (2018) Experimental USA Consensus messaging is most effective in conservative parts of the USA.  Y
29 Bertoldo et al. (2019) Correlational  UK, France, Germany, & Norway Perceived consensus predicts belief in anthropogenic climate change with the relationship moderated by whether people’s model of science is “truth” vs “debate.”  Y
30 Goldberg et al. (2019a) Experimental USA Consensus messaging reduces partisan differences on perceived consensus. Y
31 Ma et al. (2019) Experimental   Consensus messaging produces reactance among conservative dismissives. N
32 van der Linden et al. (2019a) Experimental USA Consensus messaging increased climate beliefs and attitudes, which were associated with increases in support for action. Conservatives showed greater belief updates. Y
33 van der Linden et al. (2019b) Experimental USA No evidence of psychological reactance in response to consensus messaging among Republicans, conservatives, or those with dismissive prior views. Y
34 Goldberg et al. (2019b) Experimental USA A video consensus message indirectly increased climate beliefs, climate worry, and global warming issue priority. Y
35 Rinscheid & Wüstenhagen (2019) Correlational Germany The closer people's perceived consensus is to the true level of scientific consensus, the stronger their preference for an earlier phase-out of coal-based energy Y
36 Clarke, Klas, Stevenson, & Kothe (2019) Experimental USA John Oliver video featuring 97% consensus increased climate action intentions among liberals, with no effect among conservatives. Little effect on mitigation or adaptation attitudes. Neutral
37 Sloane & Wiles (2020) Experimental USA Consensus messaging increased worry about climate change and confidence to communicate about the scientific consensus. Y

This is a quickly growing body of literature and I will continue to add to this list as more studies are published (so please don't hesitate to let me know if there are new studies or if I missed any).

References

Aklin, M., & Urpelainen, J. (2014). Perceptions of scientific dissent undermine public support for environmental policy. Environmental Science & Policy, 38, 173-177.

Bertoldo, R., Mays, C., Böhm, G., Poortinga, W., Poumadere, M., Tvinnereim, E., Arnold, A.,Steentjes, K., & Pidgeon, N. (2019). Scientific truth or debate: On the link between perceived scientific consensus and belief in anthropogenic climate change. Public Understanding of Science, https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662519865448

Bolsen, T., Leeper, T. J., & Shapiro, M. A. (2014). Doing What Others Do Norms, Science, and Collective Action on Global Warming. American Politics Research, 42(1), 65-89.

Bolsen, T. and Druckman, J.N., (2018a). Validating Conspiracy Beliefs and Effectively Communicating Scientific Consensus. Weather, Climate, and Society, 10(3), pp.453-458.

Bolsen, T., & Druckman, J. N. (2018b). Do partisanship and politicization undermine the impact of a scientific consensus message about climate change? Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 21(3), 389-402.

Brewer, P. R., & McKnight, J. (2017). “A Statistically Representative Climate Change Debate”: Satirical Television News, Scientific Consensus, and Public Perceptions of Global Warming. Atlantic Journal of Communication, 25(3), 166-180.

Clarke, E. J., Klas, A., Stevenson, J., & Kothe, E. J. (2019). The Role of Late-Night Infotainment Comedy in Communicating Climate Change Consensus. PsyArXiv. October, 4.

Cook, J. & Lewandowsky, S. (2016). Rational Irrationality: Modeling Climate Change Belief Polarization Using Bayesian Networks. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 160-179.

Cook, J., Lewandowsky, S., & Ecker, U. K. (2017). Neutralizing misinformation through inoculation: Exposing misleading argumentation techniques reduces their influence. PLoS One, 12(5), e0175799.

Deryugina, T., & Shurchkov, O. (2016). The Effect of Information Provision on Public Consensus about Climate Change. PLoS One, 11(4), e0151469.

Ding, D., Maibach, E. W., Zhao, X., Roser-Renouf, C., & Leiserowitz, A. (2011). Support for climate policy and societal action are linked to perceptions about scientific agreement. Nature Climate Change, 1(9), 462-466.

Dixon, G., Hmielowski, J., & Ma, Y. (2017). Improving Climate Change Acceptance Among US Conservatives Through Value-Based Message Targeting. Science Communication, 1075547017715473.

Goldberg, M. H., van der Linden, S., Ballew, M. T., Rosenthal, S. A., & Leiserowitz, A. (2019a). The role of anchoring in judgments about expert consensus. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, e0001.

Goldberg, M. H., van der Linden, S., Ballew, M. T., Rosenthal, S. A., Gustafson, A., Leiserowitz, A. (2019). The Experience of Consensus: Video as an Effective Medium to Communicate Scientific Agreement on Climate Change. Science Communication.

Hamilton, L. C. (2016). Public Awareness of the Scientific Consensus on Climate. SAGE Open, 6(4), 2158244016676296.

Harris, A. J., Sildmäe, O., Speekenbrink, M., & Hahn, U. (2018). The potential power of experience in communications of expert consensus levels. Journal of Risk Research, 1-17.

Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A., Bain, P. G., Fielding, K. S. (2016). Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change, DOI: 10.1038/NCLIMATE2943.

Kerr, J. R., & Wilson, M. S. (2018a). Changes in perceived scientific consensus shift beliefs about climate change and GM food safety. PloS One, 13(7), e0200295.

Kerr, J. R., & Wilson, M. S. (2018b). Perceptions of scientific consensus do not predict later beliefs about the reality of climate change: A test of the gateway belief model using cross-lagged panel analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology.

Kobayashi, K. (2018b). The Impact of Perceived Scientific and Social Consensus on Scientific Beliefs. Science Communication, 40(1), 63-88.

Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Vaughan, S. (2013). The pivotal role of perceived scientific consensus in acceptance of science. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 399-404.

Ma, Y., Dixon, G., & Hmielowski, J. D. (2019). Psychological Reactance From Reading Basic Facts on Climate Change: The Role of Prior Views and Political Identification. Environmental Communication: A Journal of Nature and Culture13(1), 71-86.

Malka, A., Krosnick, J. A., & Langer, G. (2009). The association of knowledge with concern about global warming: Trusted information sources shape public thinking. Risk Analysis, 29(5), 633-647.

McCright, A. M., Dunlap, R. E., & Xiao, C. (2013). Perceived scientific agreement and support for government action on climate change in the USA. Climatic Change, 119(2), 511-518.

Myers, T. A., Maibach, E., Peters, E., & Leiserowitz, A. (2015). Simple Messages Help Set the Record Straight about Scientific Agreement on Human-Caused Climate Change: The Results of Two Experiments. PloS One, 10(3), e0120985-e0120985.

Rinscheid, A., & Wüstenhagen, R. (2019). Germany’s decision to phase out coal by 2038 lags behind citizens’ timing preferences. Nature EnergyVol. 4, pp. 856–863, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41560-019-0460-9

Rolfe-Redding, J., Maibach, E. W., Feldman, L., & Leiserowitz, A. (2011). Republicans and climate change: An audience analysis of predictors for belief and policy preferences. Available at SSRN 2026002. [accessed 6 Feb 2019]. http://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2026002

Schuldt, J. P., & Pearson, A. R. (2016). The role of race and ethnicity in climate change polarization: evidence from a US national survey experiment. Climatic Change, 136(3-4), 495-505.

Sloane, J. D., & Wiles, J. R. (2020). Communicating the consensus on climate change to college biology majors: The importance of preaching to the choir. Ecology and Evolution.

Tom, J. C. (2018). Social Origins of Scientific Deviance: Examining Creationism and Global Warming Skepticism. Sociological Perspectives, 0731121417710459.

van der Linden, S. L., Leiserowitz, A. A., Feinberg, G. D., & Maibach, E. W. (2014). How to communicate the scientific consensus on climate change: plain facts, pie charts or metaphors? Climatic Change, 126(1-2), 255-262.

van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A. A., Feinberg, G. D., & Maibach, E. W. (2015). The scientific consensus on climate change as a gateway belief: Experimental evidence. PLoS One, 10(2), e0118489.

van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., Rosenthal, S., & Maibach, E. (2017a). Inoculating the public against misinformation about climate change. Global Challenges, 1(2), 1600008.

van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. (2018b). Perceptions of scientific consensus predict later beliefs about the reality of climate change using cross-lagged panel analysis: A response to Kerr and Wilson (2018). Journal of Environmental Psychology, 60, 110-111.

van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. (2019a). The gateway belief model: A large-scale replication. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 62, 49-58.

van der Linden, S., Maibach, E., & Leiserowitz, A. (2019b). Exposure to Scientific Consensus Does Not Cause Psychological Reactance. Environmental Communication, 1-8.

Zhang, B., van der Linden, S., Mildenberger, M., Marlon, J. R., Howe, P. D., & Leiserowitz, A. (2018). Experimental effects of climate messages vary geographically. Nature Climate Change8(5), 370.

UPDATE 10 Sep 2019: Added a new study Goldberg et al. (2019b), finding support for the Gateway Belief Model using consensus messages in video form.

UPDATE 21 Sep 2019: Added a new study Rinscheid & Wüstenhagen (2019), finding that the closer people's perceived consensus is to the true level of scientific consensus, the stronger their preference for an earlier phase-out of coal-based energy.

UPDATE 8 Oct 2019: Added a new study Clarke, Klas, Stevenson, & Kothe (2019). The conclusions of this paper are ambivalent so I categorized this paper as neutral.

UPDATE 9 Jan 2020: Added a new study Sloane & Wiles (2020), finding that consensus messaging increased worry about climate change and confidence to communicate about the scientific consensus.

Posted by John Cook on Wednesday, 7 August, 2019


Creative Commons License The Skeptical Science website by Skeptical Science is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 Unported License.